Mistakes strengthen the temporal binding effect in the context of goal-directed actions

被引:0
|
作者
Michael Jenkins
Sukhvinder S. Obhi
机构
[1] McMaster University,Social Brain, Body and Action Lab, Department of Psychology, Neuroscience and Behaviour
来源
关键词
Sense of agency; Intentional binding; Mistakes;
D O I
暂无
中图分类号
学科分类号
摘要
Temporal binding is an illusion in which the temporal interval between two events appears compressed. In the context of intentional actions, this effect is observed as a compression of the perceived interval between these actions and their causal outcomes. This ‘intentional binding effect’ has been used to investigate the Sense of Agency, which is the experience of intentionally causing an outcome through volitional action. Intentional binding is reduced for negative outcomes such as error feedback, but the role of mistakes (e.g., errors of commission) for binding and agency has not been extensively studied. In our study, participants played a virtual game in which they attempted to ‘splat’ (hit) visual stimuli that looked like coloured bugs, using mouse clicks. On some trials, stimulus colours changed unpredictably immediately before actions were made, sometimes inducing mistakes. Actions were thus clearly identifiable as mistakes at the time of their onset before any outcome feedback had been provided. Participants reported shorter action–outcome intervals when stimuli changed, but only when this change caused a mistake according to the game’s rules. This suggests that intentional binding is strengthened by errors of commission. We discuss how this effect may be accounted for by agency itself and via more general processes such as changes in arousal.
引用
收藏
页码:2191 / 2203
页数:12
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [41] Enhanced activation in the extrastriate body area by goal-directed actions
    Takahashi, Hidehiko
    Shibuya, Tomohisa
    Kato, Motoichiro
    Sassa, Takeshi
    Koeda, Michihiko
    Yahata, Noriaki
    Suhara, Tetsuya
    Okubo, Yoshiro
    PSYCHIATRY AND CLINICAL NEUROSCIENCES, 2008, 62 (02) : 214 - 219
  • [42] Synthesizing Goal-Directed Actions from a Library of Example Movements
    Ude, Ales
    Riley, Marcia
    Nemec, Bojan
    Kos, Andrej
    Asfour, Tamim
    Cheng, Gordon
    HUMANOIDS: 2007 7TH IEEE-RAS INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON HUMANOID ROBOTS, 2007, : 115 - +
  • [43] The Power of Goal-Directed Processes in the Causation of Emotional and Other Actions
    Moors, Agnes
    Boddez, Yannick
    De Houwer, Jan
    EMOTION REVIEW, 2017, 9 (04) : 310 - 318
  • [44] Bidirectional modulation of goal-directed actions by prefrontal cortical dopamine
    Hitchcott, Paul K.
    Quinn, Jennifer J.
    Taylor, Jane R.
    CEREBRAL CORTEX, 2007, 17 (12) : 2820 - 2827
  • [45] Creating novel goal-directed actions using chaotic dynamics
    Arie, Hiroaki
    Endot, Tetsuro
    Arakaki, Takafumi
    Sugano, Shigeki
    Tani, Jun
    2009 IEEE 8TH INTERNATIONAL CONFERENCE ON DEVELOPMENT AND LEARNING, 2009, : 99 - +
  • [46] Tourists' Goal-Directed Behaviors: The Influences of Goal Disclosure, Goal Commitment, and Temporal Distance
    Su, Lujun
    Yang, Xiaojie
    Huang, Yinghua
    JOURNAL OF TRAVEL RESEARCH, 2022, 61 (04) : 940 - 960
  • [47] The posterior superior temporal sulcus is sensitive to the outcome of human and non-human goal-directed actions
    Shultz, Sarah
    Lee, Su Mei
    Pelphrey, Kevin
    McCarthy, Gregory
    SOCIAL COGNITIVE AND AFFECTIVE NEUROSCIENCE, 2011, 6 (05) : 602 - 611
  • [48] EFFECT OF GOAL-DISTANCE UPON GOAL-DIRECTED WALKING
    TAKAHASHI, H
    JAPANESE JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGY, 1977, 48 (04): : 239 - 243
  • [49] The perceptual support of goal-directed displacement is context-dependent
    Bastin, J
    Montagne, G
    NEUROSCIENCE LETTERS, 2005, 376 (02) : 121 - 126
  • [50] On Using Guided Motor Primitives to Execute Continuous Goal-Directed Actions
    Morante, Santiago
    Victores, Juan G.
    Jardon, Alberto
    Balaguer, Carlos
    2014 23RD IEEE INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON ROBOT AND HUMAN INTERACTIVE COMMUNICATION (IEEE RO-MAN), 2014, : 613 - 618