How do insects represent familiar terrain?

被引:12
|
作者
Collett, TS [1 ]
Collett, M
机构
[1] Univ Sussex, Sch Biol Sci, Sussex Ctr Neurosci, Brighton BN1 9QG, E Sussex, England
[2] Michigan State Univ, Dept Zool, E Lansing, MI 48824 USA
关键词
path integration; landmark learning; resetting; global vectors; local vectors; navigation; desert ants;
D O I
10.1016/j.jphysparis.2004.03.012
中图分类号
Q189 [神经科学];
学科分类号
071006 ;
摘要
We argue here that ants and bees have a piecemeal representation of familiar terrain. These insects remember no more than what is needed to sustain the separate and parallel strategies that they employ when travelling between their nest and foraging sites. One major strategy is path integration. The insect keeps a running tally of its distance and direction from the nest and so can always return home. This global path integration is enhanced by long-term memories of significant sites that insects store in terms of the coordinates (direction and distance) of these sites relative to the nest. With these memories insects can plan routes that are steered by path integration to such sites. Quite distinct from global path integration are memories associated with familiar routes. Route memories include stored views of landmarks along the route with, in some cases, local vectors linked to them. Local vectors by encoding the direction and/or distance from one landmark to the next, or from one landmark to a goal, help an insect keep to a defined route. We review experiments showing that although local vectors can be recalled by recognising landmarks, the global path integration system is independent of landmark information and that landmarks do not have positional coordinates associated with them. The major function of route landmarks is thus procedural, telling an insect what action to perform next, rather than its location relative to the nest. (C) 2004 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:259 / 264
页数:6
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] How do herbivorous insects respond to drought stress in trees?
    Gely, Claire
    Laurance, Susan G. W.
    Stork, Nigel E.
    BIOLOGICAL REVIEWS, 2020, 95 (02) : 434 - 448
  • [32] Contact Mechanics at the Insect-Plant Interface: How Do Insects Stick and How Do Plants Prevent This?
    Gorb, Elena V.
    Gorb, Stanislav N.
    IUTAM SYMPOSIUM ON SCALING IN SOLID MECHANICS, 2009, 10 : 243 - 252
  • [33] DO REPRESENTATIVES REPRESENT
    CRANE, WW
    JOURNAL OF POLITICS, 1960, 22 (02): : 295 - 299
  • [34] Do Experiences Represent?
    Jacovides, Michael
    INQUIRY-AN INTERDISCIPLINARY JOURNAL OF PHILOSOPHY, 2010, 53 (01): : 87 - 103
  • [35] HOW AND WHY DO THEY EDUCATE THE FAMILIES TODAY? THE NEW PROCESSES OF FAMILIAR SOCIALIZATION
    Hernandez Prados, Ma Angeles
    PEDAGOGIA SOCIAL REVISTA INTERUNIVERSITARIA, 2015, (26): : 365 - 366
  • [36] How Well Do Operational Numerical Weather Prediction Configurations Represent Hydrology?
    Zsoter, Ervin
    Cloke, Hannah
    Stephens, Elisabeth
    de Rosnay, Patricia
    Munoz-Sabater, Joaquin
    Prudhomme, Christel
    Pappenberger, Florian
    JOURNAL OF HYDROMETEOROLOGY, 2019, 20 (08) : 1533 - 1552
  • [37] HOW WELL DO MWL AND CEL PREPARATIONS REPRESENT THE WHOLE HARDWOOD LIGNIN?
    Capanema, Ewellyn
    Balakshin, Mikhail
    Katahira, Rui
    Chang, Hou-min
    Jameel, Hasan
    JOURNAL OF WOOD CHEMISTRY AND TECHNOLOGY, 2015, 35 (01) : 17 - 26
  • [38] How well do general circulation models represent lowfrequency rainfall variability?
    Rocheta, Eytan
    Sugiyanto, Michael
    Johnson, Fiona
    Evans, Jason
    Sharma, Ashish
    WATER RESOURCES RESEARCH, 2014, 50 (03) : 2108 - 2123
  • [39] How accurately do force fields represent protein side chain ensembles?
    Petrovic, Dusan
    Wang, Xue
    Strodel, Birgit
    PROTEINS-STRUCTURE FUNCTION AND BIOINFORMATICS, 2018, 86 (09) : 935 - 944
  • [40] Remembering breakfast: How do pre-schoolers represent an everyday event?
    Sims, Ceri
    Morton, John
    COGNITION, 2021, 213