Moral Duty and Equalization Concerns Motivate Children Third-Party Punishment

被引:9
|
作者
Arini, Rhea L. [1 ,2 ,3 ]
Wiggs, Luci [1 ]
Kenward, Ben [1 ]
机构
[1] Oxford Brookes Univ, Ctr Psychol Res, Oxford, England
[2] Univ Oxford, Inst Cognit & Evolutionary Anthropol, Oxford, England
[3] Univ Oxford Magdalen Coll, Calleva Res Ctr Evolut & Human Sci, High St, Oxford OX1 4AU, England
关键词
children's third-party punishment behavior; affective states; audience effects; descriptive and injunctive norm violations; moral domains; JUSTICE; FOUNDATIONS; DETERRENCE; PSYCHOLOGY; NORMS; GAMES; FOCUS; FIELD;
D O I
10.1037/dev0001191
中图分类号
B844 [发展心理学(人类心理学)];
学科分类号
040202 ;
摘要
Although children enact third-party punishment, at least in response to harm and fairness violations, much remains unknown about this behavior. We investigated the tendency to make the punishment fit the crime in terms of moral domain; developmental patterns across moral domains; the effects of audience and descriptive norm violations; and enjoyment of inflicting punishment. We tested 5- to 11-year-olds in the United Kingdom (N = 152 across two experiments, 55 girls and 97 boys, predominantly White and middle-class). Children acted as referees in a computer game featuring teams of players: As these players violated fairness or loyalty norms, children were offered the opportunity to punish them. We measured the type (lining or banning) and severity of punishment children chose and their enjoyment in doing so. Children only partially made the punishment fit the crime: They showed no systematic punishment choice preference for disloyal players, but tended to fine rather than ban players allocating resources unfairly-a result best explained by equalization concerns. Children's punishment severity was not affected by audience presence or perpetrators' descriptive norm violations, but was negatively predicted by age (unless punishment could be used as an equalization tool). Most children did not enjoy punishing. and those who believed they allocated real punishment reported no enjoyment more often than children who believed they pretended to punish. Contrary to picdictions, retribution was not a plausible motive for the observed punishment behavior. Children are likely to have punished for deterrence reasons or because they felt they ought to.
引用
收藏
页码:1325 / 1341
页数:17
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [11] The Emerging Neuroscience of Third-Party Punishment
    Krueger, Frank
    Hoffman, Morris
    TRENDS IN NEUROSCIENCES, 2016, 39 (08) : 499 - 501
  • [12] Third-party punishment by preverbal infants
    Yasuhiro Kanakogi
    Michiko Miyazaki
    Hideyuki Takahashi
    Hiroki Yamamoto
    Tessei Kobayashi
    Kazuo Hiraki
    Nature Human Behaviour, 2022, 6 : 1234 - 1242
  • [13] Third-party punishment by preverbal infants
    Kanakogi, Yasuhiro
    Miyazaki, Michiko
    Takahashi, Hideyuki
    Yamamoto, Hiroki
    Kobayashi, Tessei
    Hiraki, Kazuo
    NATURE HUMAN BEHAVIOUR, 2022, 6 (09) : 1234 - +
  • [14] Third-party punishment: Retribution or deterrence?
    Tan, Fangfang
    Xiao, Erte
    JOURNAL OF ECONOMIC PSYCHOLOGY, 2018, 67 : 34 - 46
  • [15] Third-party punishment and social norms
    Fehr, E
    Fischbacher, U
    EVOLUTION AND HUMAN BEHAVIOR, 2004, 25 (02) : 63 - 87
  • [16] fMRI analysis of third-party punishment
    不详
    NEUROSCIENTIST, 2009, 15 (03): : 214 - 214
  • [17] Gratitude increases third-party punishment
    Vayness, Jonathan
    Duong, Fred
    DeSteno, David
    COGNITION & EMOTION, 2020, 34 (05) : 1020 - 1027
  • [18] Compassion Magnifies Third-Party Punishment
    Pfattheicher, Stefan
    Sassenrath, Claudia
    Keller, Johannes
    JOURNAL OF PERSONALITY AND SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY, 2019, 117 (01) : 124 - 141
  • [19] Hadza CooperationSecond-Party Punishment, Yes; Third-Party Punishment, No
    Frank W. Marlowe
    Human Nature, 2009, 20 : 417 - 430
  • [20] Can authority affect the behavior of children 's third-party punishment?
    Zhou, Shuangzhu
    Chang, Siyu
    Li, Yun
    Zhang, Jing
    CURRENT PSYCHOLOGY, 2024, 43 (44) : 34084 - 34090