Application of Threshold-bias Independent Analysis to Eye-tracking and FROC Data

被引:3
|
作者
Chakraborty, Dev P. [1 ]
Yoon, Hong-Jun
Mello-Thoms, Claudia [1 ,2 ]
机构
[1] Univ Pittsburgh, Dept Radiol, Sch Med, Pittsburgh, PA 15213 USA
[2] Univ Pittsburgh, Dept Biomed Informat, Sch Med, Pittsburgh, PA 15213 USA
关键词
Visual search; observer performance; eye-tracking; figures-of-merit; agreement; threshold-bias; OBSERVER PERFORMANCE; VISUAL-SEARCH; DECISION-MAKING; ROC CURVES; MODEL; VARIABILITY; METHODOLOGY; MAMMOGRAMS; PARADIGM; POSITION;
D O I
10.1016/j.acra.2012.09.002
中图分类号
R8 [特种医学]; R445 [影像诊断学];
学科分类号
1002 ; 100207 ; 1009 ;
摘要
Rationale and Objectives: Studies of medical image interpretation have focused on either assessing radiologists' performance using, for example, the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) paradigm, or assessing the interpretive process by analyzing their eye-tracking (ET) data. Analysis of ET data has not benefited from threshold-bias independent figures of merit (FOMs) analogous to the area under the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve. The aim was to demonstrate the feasibility of such FOMs and to measure the agreement between FOMs derived from free-response ROC (FROC) and ET data. Methods: Eight expert breast radiologists interpreted a case set of 120 two-view mammograms while eye-position data and FROC data were continuously collected during the interpretation interval. Regions that attract prolonged (>800 ms) visual attention were considered to be virtual marks, and ratings based on the dwell and approach-rate (inverse of time-to-hit) were assigned to them. The virtual ratings were used to define threshold-bias independent FOMs in a manner analogous to the area under the trapezoidal alternative FROC (AFROC) curve (0 = worst, 1 = best). Agreement at the case level (0.5 = chance, 1 = perfect) was measured using the jackknife and 95% confidence intervals (Cl) for the FOMs and agreement were estimated using the bootstrap. Results: The AFROC mark-ratings' FOM was largest at 0.734 (Cl 0.65-0.81) followed by the dwell at 0.460 (0.34-0.59) and then by the approach-rate FOM 0.336 (0.25-0.46). The differences between the FROC mark-ratings' FOM and the perceptual FOMs were significant (P<.05). All pairwise agreements were significantly better then chance: ratings vs. dwell 0.707 (0.63-0.88), dwell vs. approach-rate 0.703 (0.60-0.79) and rating vs. approach-rate 0.606 (0.53-0.68). The ratings vs. approach-rate agreement was significantly smaller than the dwell vs. approach-rate agreement (P=.008). Conclusions: Leveraging current methods developed for analyzing observer performance data could complement current ways of analyzing ET data and lead to new insights.
引用
收藏
页码:1474 / 1483
页数:10
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] Attentional bias and childhood maltreatment in clinical depression - An eye-tracking study
    Bodenschatz, Charlott Maria
    Skopinceva, Marija
    Russ, Theresa
    Suslow, Thomas
    JOURNAL OF PSYCHIATRIC RESEARCH, 2019, 112 : 83 - 88
  • [32] Application of eye-tracking in nursing research: A scoping review
    Hu, Huiling
    Li, Huijun
    Wang, Binlin
    Zhang, Mingming
    Wu, Bilin
    Wu, Xue
    NURSING OPEN, 2024, 11 (02):
  • [33] Application of Eye-tracking in research on the theory of mind in ASD
    Czajeczny, D.
    Jaroszkiewicz, A.
    Daroszewski, P.
    Kopczynski, P.
    Warchol-Biedermann, K.
    Piglowska, A.
    Samborski, W.
    Wojciak, R. W.
    Mojs, E.
    EUROPEAN REVIEW FOR MEDICAL AND PHARMACOLOGICAL SCIENCES, 2022, 26 (04) : 1364 - 1373
  • [34] Craving is everything: An eye-tracking exploration of attentional bias in binge drinking
    Bollen, Zoe
    Masson, Nicolas
    Salvaggio, Samuel
    D'Hondt, Fabien
    Maurage, Pierre
    JOURNAL OF PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY, 2020, 34 (06) : 636 - 647
  • [35] Gazepath: An eye-tracking analysis tool that accounts for individual differences and data quality
    van Renswoude, Daan R.
    Raijmakers, Maartje E. J.
    Koornneef, Arnout
    Johnson, Scott P.
    Hunnius, Sabine
    Visser, Ingmar
    BEHAVIOR RESEARCH METHODS, 2018, 50 (02) : 834 - 852
  • [36] The pottery skills and tacit knowledge of a maser: An analysis using eye-tracking data
    Nakamura, Jun
    Nagayoshi, Sanetake
    KNOWLEDGE-BASED AND INTELLIGENT INFORMATION & ENGINEERING SYSTEMS (KES 2019), 2019, 159 : 1680 - 1687
  • [37] Attentional bias toward negative stimuli in PTSD: an eye-tracking study
    Veerapa, Emilie
    Grandgenevre, Pierre
    Vaiva, Guillaume
    Duhem, Stephane
    El Fayoumi, Mohamed
    Vinnac, Benjamin
    Szaffarczyk, Sebastien
    Wathelet, Marielle
    Fovet, Thomas
    D'Hondt, Fabien
    PSYCHOLOGICAL MEDICINE, 2023, 53 (12) : 5809 - 5817
  • [38] Obsessive-compulsive symptoms and attentional bias: An eye-tracking methodology
    Bradley, Maria C.
    Hanna, Donncha
    Wilson, Paul
    Scott, Gareth
    Quinn, Paul
    Dyer, Kevin F. W.
    JOURNAL OF BEHAVIOR THERAPY AND EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHIATRY, 2016, 50 : 303 - 308
  • [39] Gazepath: An eye-tracking analysis tool that accounts for individual differences and data quality
    Daan R. van Renswoude
    Maartje E. J. Raijmakers
    Arnout Koornneef
    Scott P. Johnson
    Sabine Hunnius
    Ingmar Visser
    Behavior Research Methods, 2018, 50 : 834 - 852
  • [40] Automatic Analysis of Eye-Tracking Data for Augmented Reality Applications: A Prospective Outlook
    Naspetti, Simona
    Pierdicca, Roberto
    Mandolesi, Serena
    Paolanti, Marina
    Frontoni, Emanuele
    Zanoli, Raffaele
    AUGMENTED REALITY, VIRTUAL REALITY, AND COMPUTER GRAPHICS, PT II, 2016, 9769 : 217 - 230