Noise masking reveals channels for second-order letters

被引:12
|
作者
Oruç, I
Landy, MS
Pelli, DG
机构
[1] Univ British Columbia, Dept Psychol, Vancouver, BC V6T 1Z4, Canada
[2] NYU, Dept Psychol, New York, NY 10003 USA
[3] NYU, Ctr Neural Sci, New York, NY 10003 USA
关键词
letter identification; second-order vision; critical-band masking; scale invariance; channel switching;
D O I
10.1016/j.visres.2005.08.016
中图分类号
Q189 [神经科学];
学科分类号
071006 ;
摘要
We investigate the channels underlying identification of second-order letters using a critical-band masking paradigm. We find that observers use a single 1-1.5 octave-wide channel for this task. This channel's best spatial frequency (c/letter) did not change across different noise conditions (indicating the inability of observers to switch channels to improve signal-to-noise ratio) or across different letter sizes (indicating scale invariance), for a fixed carrier frequency (c/letter). However, the channel's best spatial frequency does change with stimulus carrier frequency (both in c/letter); one is proportional to the other. Following Majaj et al. (Majaj, N. J., Pelli, D. G., Kurshan, P., & Palomares, M. (2002). The role of spatial frequency channels in letter identification. Vision Research, 42, 1165-1184), we define 11 stroke frequency" as the line frequency (strokes/deg) in the luminance image. That is, for luminance-defined letters, stroke frequency is the number of lines (strokes) across each letter divided by letter width. For second-order letters, letter texture stroke frequency is the number of carrier cycles (luminance lines) within the letter ink area divided by the letter width. Unlike the nonlinear dependence found for first-order letters (implying scale-dependent processing), for second-order letters the channel frequency is half the letter texture stroke frequency (suggesting scale-invariant processing). (c) 2005 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.
引用
收藏
页码:1493 / 1506
页数:14
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] Letter recognition reveals pathways of second-order and third-order motion
    Ho, CE
    PROCEEDINGS OF THE NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES OF THE UNITED STATES OF AMERICA, 1998, 95 (01) : 400 - 404
  • [32] The role of second-order statistics in blind equalization of nonlinear channels
    López-Valcarce, R
    Dasgupta, S
    PROCEEDINGS OF THE TENTH IEEE WORKSHOP ON STATISTICAL SIGNAL AND ARRAY PROCESSING, 2000, : 211 - 215
  • [33] Extrafoveal viewing reveals the nature of second-order human vision
    Vakrou, Chara
    Whitaker, David
    McGraw, Paul V.
    JOURNAL OF VISION, 2007, 7 (14):
  • [34] Role of second-order statistics in blind equalization of nonlinear channels
    Lopez-Valcarce, Roberto
    Dasgupta, Soura
    IEEE Signal Processing Workshop on Statistical Signal and Array Processing, SSAP, 2000, : 211 - 215
  • [35] Second-Order Asymptotics of Covert Communications over Noisy Channels
    Tahmasbi, Mehrdad
    Bloch, Matthieu R.
    2016 IEEE INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON INFORMATION THEORY, 2016, : 2224 - 2228
  • [36] Noise gain expressions for low noise second-order digital filter structures
    Ritzerfeld, JHF
    IEEE TRANSACTIONS ON CIRCUITS AND SYSTEMS II-EXPRESS BRIEFS, 2005, 52 (04): : 223 - 227
  • [37] Noise calculations within the second-order von Neumann approach
    Zedler, Philipp
    Emary, Clive
    Brandes, Tobias
    Novotny, Tomas
    PHYSICAL REVIEW B, 2011, 84 (23)
  • [38] Impulse noise removal utilizing second-order difference analysis
    Dang, Dung
    Luo, Wenbin
    SIGNAL PROCESSING, 2007, 87 (09) : 2017 - 2025
  • [39] Second-order TGV model for Poisson noise image restoration
    Li, Hou-biao
    Wang, Jun-yan
    Dou, Hong-xia
    SPRINGERPLUS, 2016, 5
  • [40] EFFECTS OF HEADWAY DISTRIBUTIONS ON SECOND-ORDER PROPERTIES OF TRAFFIC NOISE
    BLUMENFELD, DE
    WEISS, GH
    JOURNAL OF SOUND AND VIBRATION, 1975, 41 (01) : 93 - 102