WHAT DETERMINES THE CAPACITY OF AUTOASSOCIATIVE MEMORIES IN THE BRAIN

被引:239
|
作者
TREVES, A
ROLLS, ET
机构
关键词
D O I
10.1088/0954-898X/2/4/004
中图分类号
TP18 [人工智能理论];
学科分类号
081104 ; 0812 ; 0835 ; 1405 ;
摘要
Threshold-linear (graded response) units approximate the real firing behaviour of pyramidal neurons in a simplified form, suited to the analytical study of large autoassociative networks. Here we extend previous results on threshold-linear networks to a much larger class of models, by considering different connectivities (including full feedback, highly diluted and multilayer feedforward architectures), different forms of Hebbian learning rules, and different distributions of firing rates (including realistic, continuous distributions of rates). This allows an evaluation of the main factors which may affect, in real cortical networks, the capacity for storage and retrieval of discrete firing patterns. In each case a single equation is derived, which determines both alpha(c), the maximum number of retrievable patterns per synapse, and I(m), the maximum amount of retrievable information per synapse. It is shown that: 1. Non-specific effects, such as those usually ascribed to inhibition, or to neuromodulatory afferents, alter the overall response, but do not affect the capacity for retrieval. 2. The crucial parameter which affects alpha(c) is a, the sparseness of the neural code. Results previously obtained with binary distributions of rates are shown to hold in general, namely that as a --> 0 (sparse coding) alpha(c) grows proportionally to (aln(l/a))-1, while I(m) depends only weakly on a. 3. When the coding is sparse, alpha(c) and I(m) become independent of the connectivity, and in particular the relative disadvantages of fully connected feedback networks, which are prominent with non-sparse codes, disappear. 4. The precise form of the distribution of rates, and that of the learning rule used, turn out to have rather limited effects on alpha(c) and I(m). It is to be noted, however, that when non-binary patterns are stored using nonlinear learning rules, such as those possibly modelling the action of NMDA receptors, information distortion occurs in retrieval, and results in a marked decrease of I(m). These results may be applied to help understand the organization of a specific network in the hippocampus thought to operate as an autoassociative memory.
引用
收藏
页码:371 / 397
页数:27
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [31] What Determines the Reference of Names? What Determines the Objects of Thought
    Jessica Pepp
    Erkenntnis, 2019, 84 : 741 - 759
  • [32] What is Learned Determines How Pavlovian Conditioned Fear is Consolidated in the Brain
    Leake, Jessica
    Leidl, Dana M.
    Lay, Belinda P. P.
    Fam, Justine P.
    Giles, Madeleine C.
    Qureshi, Omar A.
    Westbrook, R. Frederick
    Holmes, Nathan M.
    JOURNAL OF NEUROSCIENCE, 2024, 44 (02):
  • [33] Brain Modularity in Arthropods: Individual Neurons That Support "What" But Not "Where" Memories
    Sztarker, Julieta
    Tomsic, Daniel
    JOURNAL OF NEUROSCIENCE, 2011, 31 (22): : 8175 - 8180
  • [34] What happens in the brain while trauma survivors recall traumatic memories?
    Wessa, M
    Flor, H
    PSYCHOPHYSIOLOGY, 2002, 39 : S86 - S86
  • [35] Binary autoassociative morphological memories derived from the kernel method and the dual kernel method
    Sussner, P
    PROCEEDINGS OF THE INTERNATIONAL JOINT CONFERENCE ON NEURAL NETWORKS 2003, VOLS 1-4, 2003, : 236 - 241
  • [36] On The Capacity of Flash Memories
    Jiang, Anxiao
    Bruck, Jehoshua
    2008 INTERNATIONAL SYMPOSIUM ON INFORMATION THEORY AND ITS APPLICATIONS, VOLS 1-3, 2008, : 93 - +
  • [37] What determines BITs?
    Bergstrand, Jeffrey H.
    Egger, Peter
    JOURNAL OF INTERNATIONAL ECONOMICS, 2013, 90 (01) : 107 - 122
  • [38] What determines competence?
    Dockery, JL
    JOURNAL OF ORAL AND MAXILLOFACIAL SURGERY, 1997, 55 (11) : 1365 - 1365
  • [39] What determines handedness?
    Landis, T.
    EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF NEUROLOGY, 2017, 24 : 752 - 752
  • [40] What Determines Caring?
    Schoonover-Shoffner, Kathy
    JOURNAL OF CHRISTIAN NURSING, 2006, 23 (03) : 1 - 1