The cocktail party effect in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)

被引:5
|
作者
Mallikarjun, Amritha [1 ]
Shroads, Emily [1 ]
Newman, Rochelle S. [1 ]
机构
[1] Univ Maryland, College Pk, MD 20742 USA
关键词
Dogs; Canines; Speech perception in noise; Hearing in noise; BREED DIFFERENCES; INFANTS; DISCRIMINATION; COMPREHENSION; NOISE;
D O I
10.1007/s10071-019-01255-4
中图分类号
B84 [心理学]; C [社会科学总论]; Q98 [人类学];
学科分类号
03 ; 0303 ; 030303 ; 04 ; 0402 ;
摘要
Like humans, canine companions often find themselves in noisy environments, and are expected to respond to human speech despite potential distractors. Such environments pose particular problems for young children, who have limited linguistic knowledge. Here, we examined whether dogs show similar difficulties. We found that dogs prefer their name to a stress-matched foil in quiet conditions, despite hearing it spoken by a novel talker. They continued to prefer their name in the presence of multitalker human speech babble at signal-to-noise levels as low as 0dB, when their name was the same intensity as the foil. This surpasses the performance of 1-year-old infants, who fail to prefer their name to a foil at 0dB (Newman in Dev Psychol 41(2):352-362, 2005). Overall, we find better performance at name recognition in dogs that were trained to do tasks for humans, like service dogs, search-and-rescue dogs, and explosives detection dogs. These dogs were of several different breeds, and their tasks were widely different from one another. This suggests that their superior performance may be due to generally more training and better attention. In summary, these results demonstrate that dogs can recognize their name even in relatively difficult levels of multitalker babble, and that dogs who work with humans are especially adept at name recognition in comparison with companion dogs. Future studies will explore the effect of different types of background noise on word recognition in dogs.
引用
收藏
页码:423 / 432
页数:10
相关论文
共 50 条
  • [1] The cocktail party effect in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)
    Amritha Mallikarjun
    Emily Shroads
    Rochelle S. Newman
    Animal Cognition, 2019, 22 : 423 - 432
  • [2] The Effect of Pet Remedy on the Behaviour of the Domestic Dog (Canis familiaris)
    Taylor, Sienna
    Madden, Joah
    ANIMALS, 2016, 6 (11): : 2 - 9
  • [3] Canis familiaris (Great Dane domestic dog)
    V. Halo, Julia
    Kidd, Jeffrey M.
    TRENDS IN GENETICS, 2022, 38 (05) : 514 - 515
  • [4] Lateralised behaviour in the domestic dog, Canis familiaris
    Wells, DL
    BEHAVIOURAL PROCESSES, 2003, 61 (1-2) : 27 - 35
  • [5] Temperament and lateralization in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)
    Schneider, Luke A.
    Delfabbro, Paul H.
    Burns, Nicholas R.
    JOURNAL OF VETERINARY BEHAVIOR-CLINICAL APPLICATIONS AND RESEARCH, 2013, 8 (03) : 124 - 134
  • [6] Language preference in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)
    Amritha Mallikarjun
    Emily Shroads
    Rochelle S. Newman
    Animal Cognition, 2023, 26 : 451 - 463
  • [7] Personality traits in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)
    Svartberg, K
    Forkman, B
    APPLIED ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR SCIENCE, 2002, 79 (02) : 133 - 155
  • [8] Language preference in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)
    Mallikarjun, Amritha
    Shroads, Emily
    Newman, Rochelle S.
    ANIMAL COGNITION, 2023, 26 (02) : 451 - 463
  • [9] Directional tracking in the domestic dog, Canis familiaris
    Wells, DL
    Hepper, PG
    APPLIED ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR SCIENCE, 2003, 84 (04) : 297 - 305
  • [10] Testicular and epididymal weights in the domestic dog (Canis familiaris)
    Roy, TJ
    Prieto, L
    Gil, MC
    VETERINARY RECORD, 2002, 150 (09) : 285 - 285